Preface |
|
viii | |
|
|
1 | (24) |
|
Cognitive psychology as a science |
|
|
1 | (3) |
|
|
4 | (8) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology |
|
|
12 | (4) |
|
|
16 | (6) |
|
|
22 | (1) |
|
|
22 | (1) |
|
|
23 | (2) |
|
Visual perception: Basic processes |
|
|
25 | (28) |
|
|
25 | (1) |
|
|
25 | (5) |
|
Depth and size perception |
|
|
30 | (8) |
|
|
38 | (5) |
|
|
43 | (7) |
|
|
50 | (1) |
|
|
51 | (2) |
|
Perception, movement, and action |
|
|
53 | (30) |
|
|
53 | (1) |
|
|
54 | (4) |
|
|
58 | (4) |
|
|
62 | (1) |
|
Motion, perception, and action |
|
|
63 | (2) |
|
|
65 | (6) |
|
Perception of object motion |
|
|
71 | (8) |
|
|
79 | (2) |
|
|
81 | (2) |
|
|
83 | (36) |
|
|
83 | (1) |
|
|
84 | (4) |
|
Marr's computational theory |
|
|
88 | (8) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology approach |
|
|
96 | (3) |
|
Cognitive science approach |
|
|
99 | (7) |
|
|
106 | (10) |
|
|
116 | (1) |
|
|
117 | (2) |
|
Attention and performance limitations |
|
|
119 | (32) |
|
|
119 | (2) |
|
Focused auditory attention |
|
|
121 | (4) |
|
|
125 | (10) |
|
|
135 | (6) |
|
|
141 | (4) |
|
|
145 | (4) |
|
|
149 | (1) |
|
|
150 | (1) |
|
Memory: Structure and processes |
|
|
151 | (34) |
|
|
151 | (1) |
|
|
151 | (5) |
|
|
156 | (8) |
|
|
164 | (4) |
|
|
168 | (7) |
|
Theories of recall and recognition |
|
|
175 | (7) |
|
|
182 | (1) |
|
|
183 | (2) |
|
Theories of long-term memory |
|
|
185 | (30) |
|
|
185 | (1) |
|
Episodic and semantic memory |
|
|
185 | (2) |
|
|
187 | (4) |
|
|
191 | (1) |
|
Transfer appropriate processing |
|
|
192 | (3) |
|
|
195 | (7) |
|
|
202 | (9) |
|
|
211 | (2) |
|
|
213 | (2) |
|
|
215 | (28) |
|
|
215 | (2) |
|
|
217 | (6) |
|
|
223 | (4) |
|
|
227 | (6) |
|
|
233 | (4) |
|
|
237 | (2) |
|
Evaluation of everyday memory research |
|
|
239 | (1) |
|
|
240 | (1) |
|
|
241 | (2) |
|
Knowledge: Propositions and images |
|
|
243 | (36) |
|
|
243 | (1) |
|
What is a representation? |
|
|
244 | (3) |
|
|
247 | (1) |
|
Propositions: Objects and relations |
|
|
248 | (4) |
|
Schemata, frames, and scripts |
|
|
252 | (5) |
|
What is an image? Some evidence |
|
|
257 | (4) |
|
Propositions versus images |
|
|
261 | (6) |
|
Kosslyn's computational model of imagery |
|
|
267 | (5) |
|
The neuropsychology of visual imagery |
|
|
272 | (1) |
|
Connectionist representations |
|
|
273 | (5) |
|
|
278 | (1) |
|
|
278 | (1) |
|
Objects, concepts, and categories |
|
|
279 | (26) |
|
|
279 | (1) |
|
Evidence on categories and categorisation |
|
|
280 | (5) |
|
The defining-attribute view |
|
|
285 | (3) |
|
|
288 | (3) |
|
|
291 | (2) |
|
Explanation-based views of concepts |
|
|
293 | (3) |
|
|
296 | (1) |
|
|
297 | (4) |
|
Evaluating theories of categorisation |
|
|
301 | (1) |
|
Neurological evidence on concepts |
|
|
302 | (1) |
|
|
303 | (1) |
|
|
304 | (1) |
|
Speech perception and reading |
|
|
305 | (30) |
|
|
305 | (1) |
|
|
306 | (4) |
|
Theories of word recognition |
|
|
310 | (4) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology |
|
|
314 | (3) |
|
|
317 | (4) |
|
|
321 | (4) |
|
Routes from print to sound |
|
|
325 | (7) |
|
|
332 | (1) |
|
|
333 | (2) |
|
|
335 | (28) |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
|
335 | (7) |
|
|
342 | (3) |
|
|
345 | (6) |
|
|
351 | (10) |
|
|
361 | (1) |
|
|
362 | (1) |
|
|
363 | (30) |
|
|
363 | (1) |
|
|
363 | (2) |
|
Speech production processes |
|
|
365 | (1) |
|
Theories of speech production |
|
|
366 | (7) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology: Speech production |
|
|
373 | (2) |
|
Cognitive neuroscience: Speech production |
|
|
375 | (1) |
|
|
376 | (5) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology: Writing |
|
|
381 | (5) |
|
Speaking and writing compared |
|
|
386 | (1) |
|
|
387 | (2) |
|
|
389 | (2) |
|
|
391 | (2) |
|
Problem solving: Puzzles, insight, and expertise |
|
|
393 | (34) |
|
|
393 | (2) |
|
Early research: The Gestalt school |
|
|
395 | (4) |
|
Newell and Simon's problem-space theory |
|
|
399 | (8) |
|
Evaluating research on Puzzles |
|
|
407 | (2) |
|
Re-interpreting the Gestalt findings |
|
|
409 | (3) |
|
From puzzles to expertise |
|
|
412 | (8) |
|
Evaluation of expertise research |
|
|
420 | (1) |
|
|
421 | (3) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology of thinking |
|
|
424 | (1) |
|
|
425 | (1) |
|
|
426 | (1) |
|
|
427 | (18) |
|
|
427 | (1) |
|
|
427 | (1) |
|
General approaches to creativity |
|
|
428 | (3) |
|
Discovery using mental models |
|
|
431 | (3) |
|
|
434 | (4) |
|
Scientific discovery by hypothesis testing |
|
|
438 | (3) |
|
Evaluating problem-solving research |
|
|
441 | (2) |
|
|
443 | (1) |
|
|
444 | (1) |
|
|
445 | (30) |
|
|
445 | (2) |
|
Theoretical approaches to reasoning |
|
|
447 | (2) |
|
How people reason with conditionals |
|
|
449 | (8) |
|
|
457 | (3) |
|
|
460 | (5) |
|
Domain-specific rule theories |
|
|
465 | (2) |
|
|
467 | (2) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology of reasoning |
|
|
469 | (1) |
|
Rationality and evaluation of theories |
|
|
470 | (2) |
|
|
472 | (1) |
|
|
472 | (3) |
|
Judgement and decision making |
|
|
475 | (14) |
|
|
475 | (1) |
|
|
476 | (7) |
|
|
483 | (3) |
|
How flawed are judgement and decision making? |
|
|
486 | (1) |
|
|
487 | (1) |
|
|
488 | (1) |
|
|
489 | (24) |
|
|
489 | (1) |
|
Does affect require cognition? |
|
|
489 | (5) |
|
Theories of emotional processing |
|
|
494 | (5) |
|
|
499 | (6) |
|
Emotion, attention, and perception |
|
|
505 | (4) |
|
Conclusions on emotional processing |
|
|
509 | (2) |
|
|
511 | (1) |
|
|
512 | (1) |
|
|
513 | (14) |
|
|
513 | (1) |
|
Experimental cognitive psychology |
|
|
513 | (3) |
|
Cognitive neuropsychology |
|
|
516 | (2) |
|
|
518 | (3) |
|
|
521 | (2) |
|
Present and future directions |
|
|
523 | (1) |
|
|
524 | (1) |
|
|
525 | (2) |
Glossary |
|
527 | (12) |
References |
|
539 | (70) |
Author index |
|
609 | (15) |
Subject index |
|
624 | |